Skip to main content

Maternal–fetal attachment in pregnant Italian women: multidimensional influences and the association with maternal caregiving in the infant’s first year of life

Abstract

Background

Maternal–Fetal Attachment (MFA) describes the cognitive-representational, emotional, and behavioral aspects of the mother–fetus relationship that develops during pregnancy.

We present two studies conducted on pregnant Italian women. In Study I, we aimed to explore multifaceted associations of MFA with variables important for a healthy pregnancy (e.g., maternal mental health, the couple’s relationship). In Study II, we investigated the predictive role of MFA on observed maternal caregiving during the first months of the infant’s life.

Methods

In Study I, 113 pregnant Italian women were assessed on MFA (Maternal Antenatal Attachment Scale, MAAS), maternal depression (Beck Depression Inventory-II, BDI-II), maternal anxiety (State Trait Anxiety Inventory – State version, STAI), adjustment of the couple (Dyadic Adjustment Scale, DAS), and perceived parental care (The Parental Bonding Instrument, PBI). In Study II, 29 mother–infant pairs were followed up at 4 months to assess observational variables of maternal caregiving through the Emotional Availability Scale (EAS) and to test for an association with MFA in pregnancy.

Results

Study I showed a significant association between MFA and the quality of the couple relationship (β = .49, P < .001) and between MFA and the recall of memories of care received in childhood (β = .22, P = .025). Study II showed a predictive effect of MFA on maternal structuring observed during mother–infant interactions at 4 months of age (β = 0.36, P = .046).

Conclusion

The study points out relevant relationship contexts that might receive care and support throughout pregnancy to protect MFA. The findings also provide thoughtful insights on the role of MFA in early maternal caregiving, suggesting that MFA might be a candidate as one putative antecedent of mother–infant interaction processes.

Peer Review reports

Background

Parenting begins before birth [1]. A substantial body of research has identified pregnancy as a valuable window for maternal and fetal health [2, 3], as well as for the emergence of key determinants of parenting and the mother–infant relationship [4, 5]. This window of plasticity provides the opportunity to deliver timely programs to support expectant parents [6], but it also requires identification of the prenatal influences that promote mother–fetus bonding during pregnancy and an understanding of how expectant mothers’ emotional bonds with the their unborn infants might contribute to later variation in their caregiving practices.

The maternal–fetal attachment (MFA) was introduced for the first time in 1981 by Cranley [7] to describe the emotional bond that an expectant mother develops towards her unborn baby [8]. MFA involves maternal engagement in positive prenatal health practices and behaviors conveying caring, commitment, and interaction with the unborn child, thus emphasizing the establishment of a unique prenatal relationship [7, 9]. Several descriptions have been proposed for MFA, suggesting that the concept has a multi-dimensional nature encompassing thoughts, feelings, and dispositions to protect, interact with, and meet the needs of the fetus [7, 10, 11]. Such a prenatal bond seems to anticipate the key ingredients of the post-natal caregiving system [12], which comprises the child’s protection, comfort, and care, through parental perception and responsiveness to child’s signals, support to the exploration, and the provision of comfort in times of distress [13, 14]. Studies on maternal caregiving have underscored that the experience of a connection with the unborn child and the maternal mental representation of the fetus might set the cognitive-representational, emotional, and behavioral bases of the post-natal mother–child relationship [15]. The limited existing studies investigating the association between MFA and maternal caregiving in first years of life support a positive association of MFA with maternal sensitivity and a mother’s responsiveness to her infant [16]. However, when maternal caregiving through direct observations of mother–infant free play interactions were assessed, no significant association between MFA and maternal care were found [17], suggesting that the extant research on MFA and maternal caregiving provide findings limited by parent-reported assessment of caregiving quality and leaves the role of MFA in observable caregiving practices as an open question.

Along with the need to understand the importance of MFA in postnatal caregiving, it is of clinical relevance to identify the key influences of MFA during pregnancy to direct prenatal intervention strategies aimed at protecting and supporting MFA. So far, studies investigating the characteristics of pregnancy associated with MFA have largely suggested mutual influences between MFA and maternal mental health [18], with negative associations reported for depressive symptoms [19, 20] and anxiety [21]. The complex physical and psychological upheavals of pregnancy and the multidimensional nature of MFA still suggest further exploration of additional prenatal influences. So far, social support has been found to play a moderate role in MFA, while socio-demographic characteristics, such as parity, age, marital status, income, and education, showed weaker associations with MFA levels [22, 23]. Largely underexplored remain the relational influences in pregnancy, such as the relationship of the expectant mother with her own mother and the couple’s marital relationship. A recent review has shown that the couple’s relationship potentially affects MFA, with a positive association between a good intimate partner relationship and higher MFA [12]. In addition, throughout pregnancy, the internal representation of the relationship with the expectant mother’s own mother is progressively activated and reworked in the mind of the expectant mother to build her own new parental attitudes [24]. Unfortunately, despite such theoretical consideration and the great importance that both couple and parental relationships have during pregnancy [25], no study has attempted to explore the role of memories of received maternal care in childhood in expectant mothers’ MFA, and only very few have investigated the role of the couple’s relationship in MFA.

Overall, studies have underscored the importance of the prenatal mother–fetus bond in maternal caregiving and the establishment of a secure mother–child relationship [26]; however, studies on MFA and observed maternal caregiving are lacking. In addition, given the potential role of MFA in maternal caregiving and considering the multidimensional influences of MFA, more intensive investigations of the influences of MFA in pregnancy are needed, including a focus on the influences of relationships.

To this end, we present two studies conducted at the Department of Women’s and Childs’s Health of the Padua University Hospital and the Department of Developmental and Social Psychology of the University of Padua, Italy. The studies aimed to address important questions about the influences of MFA and its role in maternal caregiving. Study I investigated the association between MFA and several characteristics of the third trimester of pregnancy. We investigate the association between MFA, mental health indexes, and relational functioning, under the hypothesis that MFA is negatively associated with mental health symptoms (i.e., depression or anxiety) and positively associated with both the quality of couple adjustment and the recall of memories of received care in childhood. Study II is aimed at exploring the longitudinal association between third-trimester MFA and the quality of observed caregiving during free-play interactions. Here, we expect that MFA is positively associated with better caregiving qualities (i.e., maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring) observed at the fourth month of the infant’s life.

Methods

Participants

The studies were conducted at the Department of Women’s and Childs’s Health of the University Hospital of Padua and at the Department of Developmental and Social Psychology of the University of Padua, Italy.

Pregnant women in their third trimester residing in northern-east Italy and attending the Padua University Hospital (Padua, Italy) for obstetric and gynecological visits were recruited. The exclusion criteria were as follows: i) age < 18 years, ii) risk of psychiatric disorders as defined by clinical score (namely a Global Symptom Index > 65) in the Symptom Checklist 90-Revised [27], iii) insufficient Italian language proficiency, especially in reading and comprehending the self-report measures, iv) preterm delivery (< 37 weeks of gestation). A total of N = 127 pregnant women took part voluntarily in Study I; a subgroup of participants (N = 29) also agreed to be contacted for Study II, which took place after delivery.

Procedure

Pregnant women were recruited from prenatal classes and/or immediately after echography visits to the Obstetrics and Gynecological Clinic of Padua University Hospital and were invited to participate in Study I: participants who voluntarily agreed to participate in the research were invited to complete a series of validated self-report questionnaires after attending the obstetric and gynecological visit. The assessment was performed at the clinic in a quiet room, in the presence of a psychologist-in-training, who was there to answer questions and address any doubts about the research and methods. Following completion of Study I, Study II was briefly presented so that participants could decide whether to give their consent to be contacted after delivery for Study II. Participants available for Study II were contacted by telephone and were invited to visit the Laboratories of the Department of Developmental and Social Psychology of the University of Padua (Padua, Italy) for an observational assessment, as part of a broader longitudinal study [28]. This assessment was conducted when the child was 4 months of age and consisted of recording short sequences [10–15 min] of mother–infant free play interactions. For this assessment, a quiet room with age-appropriate toys was made available.

Both studies received ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board of the University of Padua, and all participants signed a written informed consent prior to enrollment. Data were collected between January 2018 and June 2019.

Measures

Maternal–Fetal Attachment (MFA)

The Maternal Antenatal Attachment Scale (MAAS) [10, 29] was used to asses MFA. It is a 19-item scale describing feelings and thoughts related to the expected child. Items are scored on a 5-point Likert scale (from 1 to 5), and the sum of all items gives a total score for maternal antenatal attachment. Two subscales are also available: the quality of mother–fetus attachment (QA) and the intensity of maternal preoccupation (MP). A test for internal consistency resulted in α = 0.72.

Maternal mental health in pregnancy

Maternal mental health during pregnancy was assessed in terms of symptoms of depression and/or anxiety. Depression was investigated through the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [30], a revised 21-item test with four response options per item (absence of a symptom [0] to severe or persistent expression of that symptom [3]). The respondent is asked to choose the statement that best reflects the way he/she has been feeling over the course of the last 2 weeks. A test for internal consistency resulted in α = 0.80. Anxiety was assessed through the State Trait Anxiety Inventory – State version (STAI) [31], a 20-item self-report questionnaire scored on a 4-point scale (1 = Not at all to 4 = Very much so). The state of anxiety refers to the experience of anxiety and tension at the time the questionnaire was completed (i.e., how the subject feels ‘now’). The total score (20–80) is given by the row sum of responses to each item, with higher scores indicating a greater state of anxiety. A test for internal consistency resulted in α = 0.93.

Couple relationship

The Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS) [32] was administered to assess the quality of the marital couple relationship. It is a 32-item self-report questionnaire assessing the degree of couple adjustment perceived by each partner. The sum of the 32 items (scored from 0 = Never agree to 5 = Always agree) gives a total score, which indicates the individual’s perceived couple adjustment, and higher scores represent greater adjustment. Cronbach’s α = 0.85 indicated a good internal consistency in this sample.

Maternal care received

The Parental Bonding InstrumentPBI Mother Form [33] is a 25-item scale measuring an individual’s retrospective perceptions of the parenting they received over the first 16 years of life. The questionnaire comprises two independent sub-scales: “Care” (12 items) and “Overprotection” (13 items). Items are scored on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 = Not at all to 3 = Very much. For the purpose of this study, the PBI-Mother Care and Over-protection scales were selected in order to study the recollection of memories about maternal caregiving.

Demographic information was collected, including mother’s age, level of education, marital status, and clinical information regarding the pregnancy.

Maternal caregiving

The Emotional Availability Scales (EAS) [34] coding system was applied to code maternal caregiving. Maternal caregiving in the context of parent–infant interactions was coded along four dimensions, namely maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring, maternal non-intrusiveness, and maternal non-hostility. Maternal sensitivity refers to clarity of perception and appropriate responsiveness to the child’s emotional expressions as well as maternal verbal and non-verbal emotional expressions, including positive, genuine and authentic affects; Maternal structuring conveys the parent’s ability to guide and scaffold the child’s exploration while maintaining emotional contact and availability; Maternal non-intrusiveness refers to general lack of over-direction, over-stimulation, over-protection, and intrusion while interacting with the child; maternal non-hostility describes the general absence of hostile responses, ranging from cover hostility (i.e., impatience, boredom) to openly hostile responses (i.e., raising the voice, becoming frightening). Each EA dimension produces score on a 7-point scale, where higher ratings standing for more optimal features. The quality of each maternal dimensions was evaluated during video-recorded free-play exchanges of mother–infant interactions when the child was 4 months of age.

Analysis plan

In Study I, correlation analyses were performed to test for associations of MFA with a series of variables of interest, such as maternal mental health in pregnancy (i.e., depression and anxiety), maternal perceived maternal care, and couple adjustment. Multivariable linear regressions were performed to test the associations among the abovementioned variables, accounting for sociodemographic (i.e., age, education level, marital status) and pregnancy-related (primiparous vs multiparous) confounding effects. In Study II, multivariable linear regression analyses were performed with maternal sensitivity, maternal structuring, maternal non-intrusiveness, and maternal non-hostility as dependent variables to test for the longitudinal association between MFA and observable dimensions of maternal caregiving during interaction at 4 months of infant life. Again, maternal socio-demographic (i.e., age, education level, marital status) and pregnancy-related characteristics (primiparous vs multiparous) were used as covariates.

In all regression analyses, one index of maternal mental health in pregnancy (either depression or anxiety) was included to avoid multicollinearity effects. The interpretation of significant effects remained unaltered; the reported results refer to models accounting for the effect of depression. As an additional control, regression analyses were performed adding nationality and employment status as extra confounding variables, and no changes in the main findings were observed. Data analyses were performed using the open-source software R [35]. Two-tailed P values were used, and results were considered significant at P < 0.05.

Results

Study I

A total of 127 expectant women (mean gestational age = 36.20 weeks; standard deviation [SD] = 3.46) were recruited for Study I. Fourteen participants were excluded according to the study criteria or for missing information exceeding 10% of responses in at least one measure. Table 1 reports the demographic characteristics of the participants in Study I. Table 2 shows descriptive statistics and correlation analysis between the study variables.

Table 1 Study I participants’ characteristics (N = 113)
Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations (95% confidence intervals)

To assess the association of MFA with maternal mental health (anxiety or depression), the couple relationship, and perceived parental care, accounting for maternal socio-demographic (i.e., age, education, marital status), and pregnancy-related characteristics (primiparous vs multiparous), multivariable linear regression analysis was performed. Regression analysis revealed significant positive associations between MFA and the quality of couple adjustment (β = 0.49 t (100) = 4.88, P < 0.001) and between MFA and the perceived level of maternal care received (β = 0.22, t (100) = 2.28, P = 0.025).

Study II

A total of 29 mothers from Study I took part in Study II; their characteristics are presented in Table 3. There were no significant differences between this sample and the women who only participated in Study I, except for a slight difference in levels of MAAS in pregnancy: Study II participants showed lower levels of MFA (p = 0.005), compared with Study I participants who did not agree to participate in Study II.

Table 3 Study II participants’ characteristics (N = 29)

The results of linear regression analyses to assess the longitudinal association of MFA with the quality of maternal caregiving in mother–infant interactions, accounting for maternal socio-demographic variables (i.e., age, education, marital status), pregnancy-related characteristics (primiparous vs multiparous), the quality of couple adjustment, the perceived level of maternal care received, and the mother’s mental health in pregnancy, are reported in Table 4.

Table 4 Multiple regression models for MFA on maternal caregiving dimensions (N = 29)

Discussion

So far, the role of maternal–fetal attachment in the quality of observed maternal caregiving has been under-investigated. This study adds to the literature in the field by highlighting: i) the multidimensional characteristics associated with MFA during pregnancy; ii) the predictive contribution of MFA to the quality of maternal caregiving observed when the infant is 4 months of age.

Study I evidenced that MFA was associated with the quality of the current romantic relationship and the levels of perceived maternal care in childhood. These findings support previous evidence showing the relational pillars of MFA [12] and confirm the importance of the couple relationship in the establishment of a healthy parent–baby relationship across the transition to parenthood [25]. Moreover, they provide the first evidence for the theoretically rooted association of MFA with the recall of memories of maternal care received in childhood. In Study I, MFA was also negatively associated with maternal anxiety during pregnancy; however, the association was no longer significant when for confounding factors were accounted for. This might be due to the low levels of maternal mental health difficulties in our sample and suggests further investigation of such contributions in more at-risk and clinical samples. Overall, Study I supports the multifaceted nature of MFA. Specifically, it gives an overview that instances of past and current relationships are active in the minds of pregnant women, perhaps modelling the maternal representations of the incoming relationship with the unborn baby.

The results of Study II show that MFA displays a positive predictive effect on maternal structuring as observed independently from behavioral mother–infant interactive exchanges at 4 months. No significant association emerged between MFA and maternal sensitivity, non-intrusiveness, or non-hostility. The specificity of the association between MFA and maternal structuring is highly important given the need for research and clinical reasons to detail the facets of the mother–infant relationship beyond the comprehensive conceptualization of maternal sensitivity. Structuring conveys the parent’s ability to guide and scaffold the child’s exploration while maintaining emotional contact and availability [34]. This ability to construct a playful setting in a way that is attuned to and will be well received by the child might tap in to emotional and cognitive and anticipatory abilities [36]. Indeed, to efficiently scaffold the interaction, parents are required to bear in mind the infant’s competencies, anticipate infant’s intentions in plausible scenarios, and control certain features of the environment in order to allow the infant to participate in aspects of play in a protected fashion. We speculate that MFA might rapidly differentiate such mothers that are already available to reflect upon their unborn baby’s signals and perhaps will be more willing to maintain such an attitude beyond the child’s birth, engaging in reflective thinking upon the infants’ intentions, capacities, affects, and thoughts while playing. Moreover, it has been observed that long-term, maternal structuring is a predictor of maternal self-esteem and the mother’s representations of herself as a parent [37]. It might be that such a process of parental identity construction finds its first steps in pregnancy, with MFA representing the first expression. That would also account for the findings of Study I, where we have observed that MFA is influenced by maternal memories of caregiving experiences in childhood, which are known to sustain maternal caregiving and infant attachment [38]. Overall, despite the limited generalizability of these findings, they validate the role of MFA in postnatal maternal care features, with the advantage of overcoming the limitation of maternal caregiving self-report evaluation. Hence, they point to the importance of prenatal maternal attachment toward the fetus as a healthy base for caregiving behaviors.

This study had some limitations. The use of self-report questionnaires in Study I should be mentioned, as it might have biased participants’ reporting on maternal mental health problems. In addition, the sample size of Study II was relatively low; this prevented us from drawing definitive and generalizable conclusion on the role of MFA in early maternal caregiving capacities and suggests the need to investigate this research question in more depth. Larger longitudinal and multi-method studies in this field are encouraged, also accounting for clinical samples, which would enable a more in-depth analysis of the association between maternal mental health in pregnancy and MFA.

Conclusion

In a novel contribution, the present study provides thoughtful insights on the potential role of MFA as a prenatal component of maternal caregiving. The importance of such findings is given by the possibility to observe in MFA the putative prenatal antecedents of such processes of mother–infant interactions that build emotion regulation and infant mental health [39]. This assumption needs to be corroborated by further studies in order to clearly delineate the continuity of prenatal-to-postnatal parenting. In addition, we identified mental health and relational features that might receive care and sustain throughout pregnancy to protect MFA and its positive effects on infant development across the transition to parenthood. Further studies ought to follow-up infants in the first years of life to explore different nuances of the early mother–infant relationship sustained by MFA.

Availability of data and materials

All data and materials support the study claims and comply with field standards. The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

MFA:

Maternal-fetal attachment

References

  1. Glover V, Capron L. Prenatal parenting. Curr Opin Psychol. 2017;15:66–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Werner E, Miller M, Osborne LM, Kuzava S, Monk C. Preventing postpartum depression: review and recommendations. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2015;18(1):41–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Wu Y, Lu Y-C, Jacobs M, Pradhan S, Kapse K, Zhao L, et al. Association of prenatal maternal psychological distress with fetal brain growth, metabolism, and cortical maturation. JAMA Netw open. 2020;3(1):e1919940–e1919940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. De Carli P, Costantini I, Sessa P, Visentin S, Pearson RM, Simonelli A. The expectant social mind: A systematic review of face processing during pregnancy and the effect of depression and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;102:153–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. HazellRaine K, Nath S, Howard LM, Cockshaw W, Boyce P, Sawyer E, et al. Associations between prenatal maternal mental health indices and mother–infant relationship quality 6 to 18 months’ postpartum: A systematic review. Infant Ment Health J. 2020;41(1):24–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Werner EA, Gustafsson HC, Lee S, Feng T, Jiang N, Desai P, et al. PREPP: postpartum depression prevention through the mother–infant dyad. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2016;19(2):229–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Cranley MS. Development of a tool for the measurement of maternal attachment during pregnancy. Nurs Res. 1981;30(5):281–4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/00006199-198109000-00008.

  8. Walsh J. Definitions matter: if maternal–fetal relationships are not attachment, what are they? Arch Womens Ment Health. 2010;13(5):449–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Salisbury A, Law K, LaGasse L, Lester B. Maternal-fetal attachment. Jama. 2003;289(13):1701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Condon JT. The assessment of antenatal emotional attachment: development of a questionnaire instrument. Psychol Psychother Theory, Res Pract. 1993;66(2):167–83.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Muller ME, Mercer RT. Development of the prenatal attachment inventory. West J Nurs Res. 1993;15(2):199–215.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. McNamara J, Townsend ML, Herbert JS. A systemic review of maternal wellbeing and its relationship with maternal fetal attachment and early postpartum bonding. PLoS One. 2019;14(7):e0220032.

  13. George C, Solomon J. Caregiving helplessness. J, Solomon, C Georg Disorganized Attach caregiving. 2011;133–66.

  14. George C. Utilising an attachment perspective in parenting assessment. Wiley Handbook what work child maltreatment an evidence based approach to assessment intervention child protection. 2017;222–38.

  15. Foley S, Hughes C. Great expectations? Do mothers’ and fathers’ prenatal thoughts and feelings about the infant predict parent-infant interaction quality? A meta-analytic review. Dev Rev. 2018;48:40–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Shin H, Park YJ, Mi JK. Predictors of maternal sensitivity during the early postpartum period. J Adv Nurs. 2006;55(4):425–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Dau ALB, Callinan LS, Smith MV. An examination of the impact of maternal fetal attachment, postpartum depressive symptoms and parenting stress on maternal sensitivity. Infant Behav Dev. 2019;54:99–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. McNamara J, Townsend ML, Herbert JS. A systemic review of maternal wellbeing and its relationship with maternal fetal attachment and early postpartum bonding. PLoS One. 2019;14(7):1–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Alhusen JL, Gross D, Hayat MJ, Rose L, Sharps P. The role of mental health on maternal-fetal attachment in low-income women. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2012;41(6):E71-81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Rubertsson C, Pallant JF, Sydsjö G, Haines HM, Hildingsson I. Maternal depressive symptoms have a negative impact on prenatal attachment–findings from a Swedish community sample. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2015;33(2):153–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Goebel A, Stuhrmann LY, Harder S, Schulte-Markwort M, Mudra S. The association between maternal-fetal bonding and prenatal anxiety: An explanatory analysis and systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2018;239:313–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Yarcheski A, Mahon NE, Yarcheski TJ, Hanks MM, Cannella BL. A meta-analytic study of predictors of maternal-fetal attachment. Int J Nurs Stud. 2009;46(5):708–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Ertmann RK, Bang CW, Kriegbaum M, Væver MS, Kragstrup J, Siersma V, et al. What factors are most important for the development of the maternal–fetal relationship? A prospective study among pregnant women in Danish general practice. BMC Psychol. 2021;9(1):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Slade A, Cohen LJ, Sadler LS, Miller M. The psychology and psychopathology of pregnancy. Handb infant Ment Heal. 2009;3:22–39.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Simonelli A, Parolin M, Sacchi C, De Palo F, Vieno A. The role of father involvement and marital satisfaction in the development of family interactive abilities: a multilevel approach. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Dayton CJ, Levendosky AA, Davidson WS, Bogat GA. The child as held in the mind of the mother: The influence of prenatal maternal representations on parenting behaviors. Infant Ment Heal J Off Publ World Assoc Infant Ment Heal. 2010;31(2):220–41.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Derogatis LR. SCL-90-R: Symptom Checklist-90-R: Administration, scoring, and procedures manual. Minneapolis: NCS Pearson; 1975.

  28. Sacchi C, De Carli P, Mento G, Farroni T, Visentin S, Simonelli A. Socio-emotional and cognitive development in Intrauterine Growth Restricted (IUGR) and typical development infants: early interactive patterns and underlying neural correlates. Rationale and methods of the study. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12(December):1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Busonera A, Cataudella S, Lampis J, Tommasi M, Zavattini GC. Investigating validity and reliability evidence for the maternal antenatal attachment scale in a sample of Italian women. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2016;19(2):329–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio. 1996;78(2):490–8.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. State-trait anxiety inventory (STAI): test manual for form X. Consulting Psychologists Press; 1968.

  32. Spanier GB. Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J Marriage Fam. 1976;15–28.

  33. Parker G. The parental bonding instrument: psychometric properties reviewed. Psychiatr Dev. 1989;7(4):317–35.

  34. Biringen Z. The emotional availability (EA) scales manual. Unpublished; 2008.

  35. R Development Core T. A language and environment for statistical computing. The R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Department of Agronomy, Faculty …; 2017.

  36. Mazursky-Horowitz H, Thomas SR, Woods KE, Chrabaszcz JS, Deater-Deckard K, Chronis-Tuscano A. Maternal executive functioning and scaffolding in families of children with and without parent-reported ADHD. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2018;46(3):463–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Biringen Z, Matheny A, Bretherton I, Renouf A, Sherman M. Maternal representation of the self as parent: Connections with maternal sensitivity and maternal structuring. Attach Hum Dev. 2000;2(2):218–32.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Verhage ML, Schuengel C, Madigan S, Fearon RM, Oosterman M, Cassibba R, et al. Narrowing the transmission gap: A synthesis of three decades of research on intergenerational transmission of attachment. Psychol Bull. 2016;142(4):337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Bornstein MH. Parenting and child mental health: a cross-cultural perspective. World Psychiatry. 2013;12(3):258–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

No financial support was received for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CS, MM and AS are responsible for the study concept and design. CS wrote the first draft of the manuscript and performed data analysis. CS, MM, SV collected the data. MM and AS contributed to the interpretation of data. AS performed study supervision. All authors critically reviewed and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chiara Sacchi.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The studies have received ethical approval from the Institutional Review Board the University of Padua (Protocol numbers: Study I: 2266/2017; Study II: 2293/2017). Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sacchi, C., Miscioscia, M., Visentin, S. et al. Maternal–fetal attachment in pregnant Italian women: multidimensional influences and the association with maternal caregiving in the infant’s first year of life. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 21, 488 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12884-021-03964-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12884-021-03964-6

Keywords